Title:
Strangers and Sex - the
role of familiarity in guppy mate choice
Author: Nirvana Naidoo
Category: UNSW
Research
Over the summer, I participated
in the UNSW
Faculty of Science Undergraduate Vacation Research Scholarship
program, open to science students in second year and above.
I completed a research project on sexual selection in guppies
with Dr Rob Brooks of the School
of Biological, Earth and Environmental Sciences, gaining
valuable practical experience as well as insight into what
is involved in scientific research.
Survival of the fittest often summons images of the strongest
or fastest. Yet even such an individual is evolutionarily
insignificant if he or she fails to pass on any of his or
her genes. Natural selection occurs because there is genetic
variation between individuals causing some, for example, to
live longer or be better at gathering food. The fittest individuals
leave the most descendants, and to do that they must be better
than their competitors at finding resources and escaping predators.
In many species, however, we find traits that do not increase
an individual’s chance of surviving to reproduce but may in
fact hinder it. Traitsm such as the brightly coloured and
ornate tails of some male birds and fish, make them easier
targets for predators. Yet these traits persist.
The
theory of natural selection cannot explain the existence of
seemingly exaggerated traits, such as the peacock’s tail,
leading Darwin to the idea of sexual selection, a special
case of natural selection. Under sexual selection, phenotypic
variation in a trait in one of the sexes, usually male, results
in differential mating success. Sexual selection can occur
either by intrasexual selection, where members of the one
sex compete for mates, usually in the form of male fights
or contests, or intersexual selection (mate choice) where
certain individuals are found to be more attractive than others.
In either case, some individuals leave more descendants than
other individuals.
The guppy,
Poecilia reticulata, is a small tropical fish, from
the freshwater streams of Trinidad, known for their conspicuous
and immensely varied male colour patterns. Mature males possess
larger tails than females and a variety of polymorphic
markings while females are of uniform colour. This sexual
dimorphism (difference) is often indicative of sexual selection.
It is
well established that male colour patterns are highly heritable
and the basis of mate choice. Males court females incessantly,
but females primarily choose whom to mate with. As a trait
under sexual selection, the level of variation in male colour
patterns is especially surprising as selection is expected
to deplete variation, as only the males with few most appealing
patterns will procreate. However, though their conspicuous
colour patterns were more than likely the attribute that initially
led to sexual selection studies in guppies, we are still unable
to satisfactorily explain why such extreme levels of polymorphism
persist.
In
the simplest scenario, the balance between mutations introducing
new variants and natural selection removing other variants
determines the level of genetic variation. Antagonistic effects
caused by conflicting forms of selection may maintain greater
than expected variation as the trade-off could result in different
combinations of variants of equal fitness. This is thought
to be the case in guppies as it is well established that brightly
coloured male guppies are favoured by females but are also
at greater risk from predators. Frequency-dependent mate choice
by females, where they prefer rare or novel (infrequent) males,
is a common proposal because it has the potential to maintain
extensive variation. The suggestion is that females prefer
males who have unusual colour patterns, a phenomenon known
as the “rare-male effect”.
There
have been few studies on frequency-dependent mate choice in
guppies. Farr1 and Hughes et
al.2 both found
a female preference for novel or unfamiliar (‘unfamiliar’
meaning new to the female but not necessarily rare in nature)
males in guppies, while Kelley. et al3
found a male preference for unfamiliar females. We tested
for preferences for an unfamiliar mate as a factor contributing
to the maintenance of the variation in male colour patterns.
A preference for unfamiliar males is argued to be equivalent
to a preference for rare males. Though female mating preferences
in guppies are considered more significant, a male preference
for unfamiliar females would also contribute to high levels
of variation.
Our
experimental protocol was similar to that of Hughes et
al.2 We familiarized
males and females and then conducted behaviour trials with
familiar and unfamiliar individuals. Female responses to male
sigmoid displays were scored according
to how much interest the female seemed to show. This method
allowed us to compare the number of displays by the unfamiliar
and familiar males to determine if males preferred unfamiliar
females. It also allowed us to compare the scores of the displays
to determine if females preferred unfamiliar males.
Based
on earlier studies, we expected to find a female preference
for unfamiliar males due to the ‘rare-male effect’. Yet, we
found no significant difference in female response to unfamiliar
and familiar males. Since none of our fish were allowed to
mate prior to the trials, we were less surprised to find no
male preference for unfamiliar females as there was no biological
advantage for males discriminating against females they have
not previously mated with. These results indicated that familiarity
alone was not important in mate choice. Thus a preference
for unfamiliar mates could not explain the extreme variation
in male colour patterns. Alternatively, it is possible that
we did not allow sufficient time for the fish to become familiarized.
However, it is unlikely that wild populations of guppies would
experience such an intensive familiarization. Thus, it would
still be unlikely that familiarity is an important component
of mate choice. In either case, a preference for unfamiliar
mates cannot explain the extreme variation in male colour
patterns.
This result
suggests the ‘rare-male effect’ may not be as significant
as once believed since the quantity of variation in itself
means every male is likely to be rare under some criteria.
So, my summer’s effort did not yield an earthshaking discovery.
However, I did learn that careful experimentation, like what
I was involved in, can challenge specialised scientific theories.
References
1. Farr, J. A.
(1977). Male rarity or novelty, female choice behaviour, and
sexual selection in the guppy, Poecilia reticulata. Evolution,
31, 162-168
2. Hughes, K.
A., Du, L., Rodd, F. H. and Reznick, D. N. (1999). Familiarity
leads to female preference for novel males in the guppy, Poecilia
reticulata. Animal Behaviour, 58,
907-916
3. Kelley, J
L., Graves, J. A. and Magurran, A. E. (1999). Familiarity
breeds contempt in guppies. Nature, 401,
661-662.
Further reading
Brooks,
R. (2002). Variation in female mate choice within guppy populations:
population divergence, multiple ornaments and the maintenance
of polymorphism. Genetica, 116,
343-358
Houde,
A. E. (1997). Sex, Color and Mate Choice In Guppies. Princeton
University Press
Glossary
Heterozygote:
A diploid organism with two different alleles of the one gene.
Homozygote:
A diploid organism with two of the same alleles of the one
gene.
Phenotype:
The physical appearance resulting from the genotype.
Polymorphism:
Variants, in this case, in the colour patterns of the male
guppies.
Sigmoid
display:
The mating displays male guppies perform to attract mates.
|