![]() | ||||
![]() |
|
|||
Why
do we say the things we do? Imagine the linguistic mind as a network of wires connecting light bulbs to one another. These light bulbs are ‘nodes’ that represent words, such as ‘cat’, or the sounds that form the word, such as ‘c’. Activation spreads from one ‘node’ to another, so when one light bulb switches on, light bulbs connected to it will switch on, too. The level of activation, or the brightness of each light bulb, indicates the extent of that node participating in the word production. Several nodes can also be active at the same time, and activation can cascade either in a top-down (meaning-word-sound) or bottom-up (sound-word-meaning) direction.4 Dell’s model can help explain mixed errors, those that share both sound features and meaning with the intended word (see Fig. 2).
When the ‘cat’ light bulb switches on, so do the light bulbs for the semantic meanings and the sounds that form the word ‘cat’. So, the words ‘dog’ and ‘rat’ are activated since they share a semantic domain with ‘cat’. However, as ‘rat’ shares sounds with ‘cat’ whereas ‘dog’ does not, the ‘rat’ light bulb is brighter than ‘dog’. Therefore, you are more likely to say the word ‘rat’ than the word ‘dog’.5 Top-down and bottom-up connections may be forming a type of filter that screens out non-words.4 This filtering might explain why speech errors to be real words. If sound patterns are stored in the lexicon, it is possible that impossible sound sequences are being filtered out as well. Bottom-up processing can help explain certain sound-based speech errors. For example, you may have problems articulating the correct sound when there is ‘competition’ between phonemes or syllables. This is what makes tongue-twisters, such as “she sells sea-shells on the seashore”, so difficult to say. Also, speech errors tend to occur more frequently when people neglect to “think before they speak”.3
Although recording speech errors has been a useful method of determining what types of errors occur whilst speaking, the data may not be entirely accurate. For instance, the listener could misinterpret what has been said. Problems can arise when the speaker and listener use different dialects; vowels may be mistaken for one another, and the same applies for consonants. There is also a suggestion that in speech error collections, there is a bias toward detecting errors located at the beginning of the word.6 Furthermore, Levelt and his colleagues have challenged the dependency on speech error data, arguing that only reaction-time studies can provide reliable evidence for word production models.7 They proposed the alternative computational model, WEAVER++ (Word-form Encoding by Activation and VERification). The design is very similar to Dell’s model, the difference being that it is able to predict the speed of word production. Nevertheless,
speech error analysis has been useful. Identifying the
situations where these errors are produced has made it
possible to develop models to explain the processes involved
in word production. Although studying slips-of-the-tongue
alone is incomplete, it has provided valuable insight
into the way words are generated in everyday speech.
1. Dell, G.S., Reed, K.D., Adams, D.R., & Meyer, A.S. (2000). Speech Errors, Phonotactic Constraints, and Implicit Learning: A Study of the Role of Experience in Language Production. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26, 1355–1367. 2. Harley, T.A., & MacAndrew, S.B.G. (2001). Constraints Upon Word Substitution Speech Errors. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 30, 394–417. 3. Eysenck, M.W., & Keane, M.T. (2001). Cognitive Psychology, 4th Edition. Hove, UK: Psychology Press, Taylor & Francis. 4. Schwartz, M.F., Saffran, E.M., Bloch, D.E. & Dell, G.S. (1994). Disordered Speech Production in Aphasic and Normal Speakers. Brain and Language, 47, 52–88. 5. Levelt, W.J.M. (1999). Models of word production. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 3, 223–232. 6. Cutler, A. (1981). The reliability of speech error data, Linguistics, 19, 560–582. 7. Levelt, W.J.M., Roelofs, A., & Meyer, A.S. (1999). A theory of lexical access in speech production. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 1–75.
Harley, T.A. (2001). The psychology of language: From data to theory, 2nd Edition. Hove, UK: Psychology Press, Taylor & Francis. OnSET is an initiative of the Science Communication Program URL: http://www.onset.unsw.edu.au/ Enquiries: onset@unsw.edu.au Authorised by: Will Rifkin, Science Communication Site updated: 7 Febuary, 2006 © UNSW 2006 | Disclaimer |
OnSET is an online science magazine, written and produced by students. OnSET Issue 6 launches for O-Week 2006!
Worldwide
Day in Science ![]() Sunswift
III
Outreach
Centre for Sciences
South
Pole Diaries |