Why
do we say the things we do?
Ada
Tam
“Please
pass me the pork… I mean fork.” Slips-of-the-tongue can
be embarrassing. However, by analysing the types of errors
and when they are produced, researchers have gained insight
into how they occur and a better understanding of the
mechanisms that underlie word production.
Have
you ever noticed that speech blunders tend to be real
words or at least sound like one? Research has shown that
speech errors seem to obey the “rules of the language”
that determine what sounds like a real word and what does
not.1 People learn
through experience about sound patterns that occur in
language. These patterns are stored in each person’s lexicon,
which acts as a ‘mental dictionary’. Guidance provided
by the lexicon makes uttering ‘impossible’ sound combinations
unlikely since the sounds are unfamiliar. Sound-based
errors are also more likely to occur in longer and infrequently
used words.
Archibald
Spooner was famous for making classic speech blunders,
such as “You have hissed all my mystery lectures”. Spoonerism
is an example of how speech errors can be real words.
Another way can involve problems in selecting the correct
word. For instance, you might say, “Where is my tennis
bat?” instead of, “Where is my tennis racquet?”2
One possible explanation is that abstract
words that are difficult to form a mental image of are
more likely to be replaced by words with easier to form
mental images.
A dominant model of word production
Dell proposed a model that describes word selection
as a two-step process (see Fig. 1).3
Fig.
1. Basic hierarchical structure of Dell's model |
Imagine
the linguistic mind as a network of wires connecting light
bulbs to one another. These light bulbs are ‘nodes’ that
represent words, such as ‘cat’, or the sounds that form
the word, such as ‘c’. Activation spreads from one ‘node’
to another, so when one light bulb switches on, light
bulbs connected to it will switch on, too. The level of
activation, or the brightness of each light bulb, indicates
the extent of that node participating in the word production.
Several nodes can also be active at the same time, and
activation can cascade either in a top-down (meaning-word-sound)
or bottom-up (sound-word-meaning) direction.4
Dell’s
model can help explain mixed errors, those that share
both sound features and meaning with the intended word
(see Fig. 2).

Fig. 2. A simplified diagram of Dell's model. Adapted
from Levelt, J.M. (1999). Models of word production,
Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 3,
p. 226
|
When
the ‘cat’ light bulb switches on, so do the light bulbs
for the semantic meanings and the sounds that form the
word ‘cat’. So, the words ‘dog’ and ‘rat’ are activated
since they share a semantic domain with ‘cat’. However,
as ‘rat’ shares sounds with ‘cat’ whereas ‘dog’ does not,
the ‘rat’ light bulb is brighter than ‘dog’. Therefore,
you are more likely to say the word ‘rat’ than the word
‘dog’.5
Top-down
and bottom-up connections may be forming a type of filter
that screens out non-words.4
This filtering might explain why speech errors to be real
words. If sound patterns are stored in the lexicon, it
is possible that impossible sound sequences are being
filtered out as well.
Bottom-up
processing can help explain certain sound-based speech
errors. For example, you may have problems articulating
the correct sound when there is ‘competition’ between
phonemes or syllables. This is what makes tongue-twisters,
such as “she sells sea-shells on the seashore”, so difficult
to say. Also, speech errors tend to occur more frequently
when people neglect to “think before they speak”.3
How useful is this model?
Although
recording speech errors has been a useful method of determining
what types of errors occur whilst speaking, the data may
not be entirely accurate. For instance, the listener could
misinterpret what has been said. Problems can arise when
the speaker and listener use different dialects; vowels
may be mistaken for one another, and the same applies
for consonants. There is also a suggestion that in speech
error collections, there is a bias toward detecting errors
located at the beginning of the word.6
Furthermore,
Levelt and his colleagues have challenged the dependency
on speech error data, arguing that only reaction-time
studies can provide reliable evidence for word production
models.7 They proposed
the alternative computational model, WEAVER++
(Word-form Encoding
by Activation and VERification).
The design is very similar to Dell’s model, the difference
being that it is able to predict the speed of word production.
Nevertheless,
speech error analysis has been useful. Identifying the
situations where these errors are produced has made it
possible to develop models to explain the processes involved
in word production. Although studying slips-of-the-tongue
alone is incomplete, it has provided valuable insight
into the way words are generated in everyday speech.
See
OnSET's The
science of reading
Glossary
Coda syllable:
Phonetic sound at the end of the word
Lexicon:
A store of detailed information about words
Onset syllable:
Phonetic sound at the beginning of the word
Phoneme:
A basic speech sound conveying meaning
Spoonerism:
A type of speech error that occurs when the sounds at
the beginning of words are exchanged and the resulting
errors are real words
References
1.
Dell, G.S., Reed, K.D., Adams, D.R., & Meyer, A.S.
(2000). Speech Errors, Phonotactic Constraints, and Implicit
Learning: A Study of the Role of Experience in Language
Production. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning,
Memory, and Cognition, 26, 1355–1367.
2.
Harley, T.A., & MacAndrew, S.B.G. (2001). Constraints
Upon Word Substitution Speech Errors. Journal of Psycholinguistic
Research, 30, 394–417.
3.
Eysenck, M.W., & Keane, M.T. (2001). Cognitive Psychology,
4th Edition. Hove, UK: Psychology Press, Taylor &
Francis.
4.
Schwartz, M.F., Saffran, E.M., Bloch, D.E. & Dell,
G.S. (1994). Disordered Speech Production in Aphasic and
Normal Speakers. Brain and Language, 47,
52–88.
5.
Levelt, W.J.M. (1999). Models of word production. Trends
in Cognitive Sciences, 3, 223–232.
6.
Cutler, A. (1981). The reliability of speech error data,
Linguistics, 19, 560–582.
7.
Levelt, W.J.M., Roelofs, A., & Meyer, A.S. (1999).
A theory of lexical access in speech production. Behavioral
and Brain Sciences, 22, 1–75.
Further Reading
Harley,
T.A. (2001). The psychology of language: From data to
theory, 2nd Edition. Hove, UK: Psychology Press, Taylor
& Francis.
|